Friday, March 30, 2012

CHOICE




“Each day I am closer and closer to an understanding.

Because every exit is an open door. Every going out
then moving in is for some brief moment
a single event. Step now, and suffer. A street
branches out to a street. What does it mean
to have a CHOICE?”

~ “Epiphany,” by Raymond de Borja

Common sense tells us that all the steps we’ve made so far in this life is but a function of our intentions. That the object of our intentions and our intentions themselves are bound to be one and the same. Could an alternate frame of thinking actually exist where our intentions are independent of our actions and choices?
Life is a one way street, they say. That the only way of moving on and making sense of life is forward destined. That intentions must precede actions. That choices are defined before making the choice itself. Paradoxical as it may sound, a new phenomenon in experimental psychology is clamoring for the possibility of intentions being defined after the choice was made. Johansson and colleagues (2005) called it CHOICE BLINDNESS.

Choice blindness stemmed from an interest to call for research targeted towards the evaluation of the importance of intention, action and introspection in perception. Its umbrella phenomenon, change blindness, was exhaustively researched for years but surprisingly lacked any relevant information related to tasks of practical and subjective importance to us.

Researchers of change blindness (Rensink, 2002; Triesch et al., 2003 in Johansson, Hall & Sikstrom, 2008) have proven that we are more likely to detect change when the parts or elements that were changed are relevant to the scene, of central interest to us, or  something where we have prior knowledge. Since choice blindness also involves changing stimuli, it would be logical to assume that it follows the same trend. It would be easier to detect changes in more familiar objects than novel or unfamiliar ones.
To better understand how change blindness works, our group (De Guzman, Molina, Orbe, & Rivera, 2012) made a small experimental attempt situating choice manipulation detection as a dependent variable of two independent variables: 1) IMAGE TYPE & 2) BACKGROUND COLOR. The former to differentiate detection rates between a familiar stimuli (faces) and a novel one (abstract images) and the latter to know the extent of the influence of peripheral stimuli (i.e. colors).
The experiment goes like this: Individuals were asked to choose one from every photo pair (5 pairs of faces and 5 pairs of abstract images) which they find more attractive or appealing. After which, they were shown once again their choice and were requested to explain the reason for the decision. At some trials, however, their original choice was manipulated; that is, they were presented the unchosen photograph instead.

About 56% of the participants failed to detect that their choice was altered. This decreased to 37% when the participants were explicitly asked whether or not something changed during the experiment. In particular, only about 17% of them were able to detect change when face photographs were used, and this increased to 50% when abstract photographs were presented. The huge discrepancy could be accounted for by the top-down processing of face photos, and the bottom-up processing of abstract photos. Since facial perception involves a holistic discernment of component parts, lesser attention may have been paid to its details. On the contrary, non-face images are susceptible to part decomposition, thus eliciting greater attention to perceive unfamiliar patterns.

Color was also found to influence the rate of change detection, but its effects are limited to the face photographs. Again, since analysis of faces is global, effects of background elements are more pronounced. Conversely, isolation or localization of elements in abstract images because of the lack of configural processing might have moderated the influence of colors in the rate of detection.

Having made to believe they made a choice they actually did not lead to the participants’ failure to recall their original choice. The time they spent looking at their manipulated choice while reasoning out increased their attention to its details, thereby enhancing the encoding process (Goldstein, 2010; Chun & Turk-Browne, 2007). Thus, the increase of the recall probability of images they actually did not choose but reasoned out for.

The perceived attractiveness or aesthetic appeal between the original choice and manipulated choice, nonetheless, were insignificantly different. This was argued to be because of the high similarity between every pair of photographs used.

Although not without its shortcomings, our experiment’s main point goes to show that prior knowledge or relevance does not necessarily lead to easier detection of change, at least in setups where active choice is involved. In fact, familiarity might have triggered top-down holistic approaches of viewing that even lessened the chances of detecting the manipulation. Furthermore, introspection evidenced through verbal explanation of preference still challenges the idea of the precedence of intention attributions before actions or choices are done.

***

“Every exit is an open door...”
Maybe change is not too bad. Maybe an ending does not mean stopping. Maybe, just maybe, standing by a choice matters more than the mere act of choosing.
Whether decisions lead to blind spots because you have done them in familiar circumstances as the results of the experiment suggests, or it leads you to be more attentive because of its relative novelty, it all boils down to the person you are and the person you want to become. In this world where the value of mistakes are underrated and the drive for external success is hyped, we can always take refuge in the fact that these “branching streets” are not meant to be walked alone. Of all the choices our goal-directed behaviors will lead to, perhaps the most important will be the choice of keeping a good set of peers by our side. 


"Friendship is unnecessary... It has no survival value; rather it gives value to survival." ~C.S. Lewis

References
Chun, M. M. & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2007). Interactions between attention and memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology,17, 177-184.
De Guzman, K., Molina, T., Orbe, D., & Rivera, L. (2012). Effects of the type of image and the type of color background on choice blindness. Unpublished manuscript.
Goldstein, E. B. (2010). Sensation and Perception (8th ed.). CA: Wadsworth.
Johansson, P., Hall, L. & Sikstrom, S. (2008). From change blindness to choice blindness. Psychologia, 51, 142-155.
Johannson, P., Hall, L., Sikstrom, S. & Olsson, A. (2005). Failure to detect mismatches between intention and outcome in a simple decision task. Science, 310, 116-119.

Tuesday, March 6, 2012

Eyow po. Ano shabi mo? :)

Hindi man halata sa akin, mahilig ako sa bata. :) Laging sinasabi ng mga tiyahin ko na ako yung tipong nasa tabi lang kapag kailangang bantayan ang mga bulilit na pinsan. Wala daw akong pakialam. Gusto ko nang sabihin na, "Tita, mahal ko din naman po yung pinsan ko. Gusto ko din s'yang alagaan." Ang ayoko lang kasi, yung madami kaming nag-aagawan sa bata. Baka naman lalong ma-windang yung bata na palipat-lipat s'ya ng brasong nagbubuhat sa kanya. Kung ako yung batang iyon, siguro umiyak na ako nang umiyak. Hindi naman siguro kasi yun komportable para sa kanya. Kawawa naman 'di ba?

Ilan sa mga pinakagusto ko sa pakikipagsalamuha sa mga maliliit na bata ay ang paglalaro ng "Eat Bulaga!", "Close, Open", at "Sawsaw Suka". Natutuwa din sa ako sa tuwing nakikipag-usap sa kanila. Nakakatuwa kasi   kung makatingin ang bata o sanggol sa iyo na parang naiintindihan n'ya lahat ng sinasabi mo, parang ito...


At sa tuwing humaharap at kumakausap ako ng sanggol, sadyang tumataas ang boses ko at nagbubulol ang mga salitang sinasabi ko. Baby talk ang tawag dito at naniniwala akong hindi ako nag-iisang umaakyat ang boses kapag may kaharap na baby. :)

Matagal nang pinag-uusapan kung kailangan ba talagang gumamit ng Baby talk ng isang tagapag-alaga sa isang sanggol at kung ano ang nagiging epekto nito sa kanyang paglaki.

Ang laging nakukuha kong rason sa mga nagbabantay ng bata na kaya sila gumagamit ng Baby talk ay nakakatawag daw ito ng atensyon ng mga sanggol. Subalit ang haka-hakang ito nang naobserbahan nila Singh at mga kasama (2009) na ang atensyon ng mga bata ay naibibigay sa mas may positive affect, kahit ang pamamaraan ng pagsasalita ay pangkaraniwan o baby talk.

Napag-aralan din nila Dehaene-Lambertz at mga kasama (2002) ang mga bahagi ng utak na aktibo sa speech perception sa ganitong yugto ng buhay. Mula sa ilang fMRI na ginawa sa mga sanggol na tatlong buwan pa lamang at malayong-malayo pa sa pagsasalita, nakita na aktibo na ang mga bahagi na kasangkot sa speech ng mga adult tulad ng superior temporal at angular gyri at ng pro-frontal cortex para sa gising na mga sanggol.

Ang galing 'di ba? Ganun kaaga na na-activate ang mga bahaging iyon para sa speech perception natin. Kaya siguro unti-unting naiintindihan ng mga sanggol ang mga pinagsasabi natin at natututo din silang magsalita habang sila ay lumalaki. :) Nakakatuwang malaman ang mga ganitong bagay, kung paano tayo natututo habang lumilipas ang panahon. :)

At dahil ang ku-kyut ng mga sanggol, hayaan nyong tapusin ko ito sa pamamagitan ng kambal na ito na kung mag-usap at magtalunan ay parang alam na ang lahat. :)



Sanggunian:
Singh, L, Morgan, J, & Best, C. Infants' Listening Preferences: Baby Talk or Happy Talk? Infancy. 3(3). 2002. Retreived from: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1207/S15327078IN0303_5. 05 March 2012.

Dehaene-Lambertz, G, Dehaene, S, & Hertz-Pannier, L. Functional Neuroimaging of Speech Perception in Infants. Science 6. 298(5600). 2013-2015. December 2002. Retrieved from: http://www.sciencemag.org/content/298/5600/2013.short. 05 March 2012.

Ang larawan ay mula sa Baby, at ang video ay mula sa Twins.

Monday, March 5, 2012

From Face to Hands


We read facial expressions, and we understand them. Matsumoto & Hwang (2011) have distinguished seven basic emotions and their corresponding facial expressions, and argued that these have biological and genetic support. Such emotions include joy, surprise, contempt, sadness, anger, disgust, and fear.

How 'bout we express these through touch?*
Aside from being able to read them, we express them ourselves. Indeed, it is not too difficult to comprehend facial expressions as a language. After all, we use this language everyday even without us being much aware of it.


Yet there’s another language we use; a language we communicate to a more limited number of recipients; a language that sometimes tells the message directly, but more often indirectly; a language that, to some extent, is harder to express and interpret: the language of touch.

It seems quite obvious that the meaning attributed to touching depends on who touches whom. A person whose hands were held by someone special would cause butterflies in his or her stomach. But it would be a different thing if his or her hands were held by a stranger.

Conversely, even when we mean the same thing, the manner by which we express it depends on whom we would touch. We may give a brief tap at the shoulder of an acquaintance in distress. We may hug a friend who’s feeling the same.

Expression and interpretation of touch depends on who we are. They depend on our disposition at the moment we are being touched. They depend on our age. They depend on our personality.

The language of touch is so interesting in that it could not be simply confined into rules for ease of expression and interpretation. Also in spite of it, we still learn to use the language accurately in most of our dealings.
***


Some of the basic emotions with universal facial expressions could be expressed accurately through touch. Such included fear, disgust, and anger. In addition to this are the prosocial emotions: love, gratitude, sympathy, surprise, happiness, and sadness. However, self-focused emotions- embarrassment, envy, pride- were said to be more difficult to express through touch (Hertenstein, Keltner, App, Bulleit, and Jaskolka, 2006; in Thomson & Hampton, 2011).

Thomson & Hampton (2011) extended the study done by Hertenstein and colleagues, and found that the longetivity of a relationship was not a good predictor of the accuracy of expression and interpretation of an emotion through touch, although romantic partners performed better at decoding different types of emotions than strangers do.

It is interesting to note also that both relationship types- partner and stranger- more often agree on the means of expressing particular emotions. For instance, anger was generally expressed by hitting the other person; disgust by pushing; fear by squeezing; happiness by lifting the hand; sadness by stroking; and surprise by also lifting. Gratitude was expressed by a handshake; love by a gentle stroke; sympathy also by a stroke; and embarrassment and envy by a squeeze.

The manner of expression of different emotions may be the same. And so it is necessary to emphasize the importance of other cues in fully and accurately understanding the meaning of every action.
***

It is amazing to realize how we speak in a lot of ways. It is even more amazing to think we are able to understand all these complex things. Really, the face, the hands, and the brain (of course)  are amazing!


*Matsumoto, D. & Hwang, H. S. (2011). Reading facial expressions of emotion: Basic research leads to training programs that improve people’s ability to detect emotions. Retrieved from Psychological Science Agenda,  American Psychological Association. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/science/about/psa/2011/05/facial-expressions.aspx

Thompson, E. H. & Hampton, J. A. (2011). The effect of relationship status on communicating emotions through touch. Cognition and Emotion, 25 (2), 295- 306.

Touching-Touching



Nasubukan mo na bang mahawakan ng isang taong di mo kakilala? Eh ng taong mahal mo? Alin ang mas gusto mo? Aminin. Halata naman eh. Nakakatakot kaya kung hahawakan ka ng isang taong di mo naman kilala.

Sa pag-ibig may iba’t ibang paraan tayong ginagawa upang maipakita ang pagmamahal. Nariyan na ang pagsambit ng I love you o Mahal kita; pagkakaroon ng term of endearment tulad ng babe, mahal, “mhine” at “bheybie qowh” sa mga jejemon, tart, cupcake, honey at kung anu-ano pang pagkaing matatamis (Pwede siguro ang bibingka o biko para naman Pinoy na Pinoy sa pandinig!); pagbibigay ng regalo at paghahanda ng surpresa.


*
Siyempre, hindi mawawala ang mga ekspresyong nagpapakita ng physical intimacy tulad ng paghalik (bahala na kayong mag-imagine kung sa cheeks o lips), pagyakap, paghawak, paghaplos, pagpisil-pisil (bahala na rin kayong mag-isip kung ano ang pinipisil) at pagkiliti na masarap gawin kapag tulog ang kinikiliti. =D

Ngunit ang mga gawaing pisikal na nabanggit ay hindi lamang natin ginagawa sa romantic relationships. Ginagamit natin ang mga ito sa pag-convey ng mga emosyon sa iba pang tao, kaibigan, kapamilya, kapuso, kabarkada o estranghero man--maliban na lamang siguro sa paghalik lalo na sa lips.

*
Ilang pag-aaral na ang isinagawa tungkol sa pagko-communicate ng iba’t ibang emosyon gamit ang touch tulad ng pag-aaral nina Hertenstein, Keltner, App, Bulleit, & Jaskolka (2006) at Hertenstein, Holmes, McCullough, & Keltner (2009) na parehong strangers ang ginawang kalahok. Ang mga sumusunod ang natukoy na type of touch sa pag-aaral na ito: finger interlocking, handshake, hitting, lifting, massaging, patting, picking, pinching, poking, pressing, pulling, pushing, rubbing, scratching, shaking, slapping, squeezing, stroking, swinging, tapping, tickling, tossing, trembling.

Ang mga ito rin ang ginamit sa pag-aaral nina Thompson & Hampton (2011) ngunit sa pagkakataong ito ay magsing-irog :)) ang mga kalahok. Inalam nila kung gaano ka-accurate ang paghula sa emosyon na isasagawa sa kapareha o sa hindi kakilala sa pamamagitan ng touch. Ang magsasagawa ng emosyon sa pamamagitan ng touch ay tinawag na encoder samantalang decoder naman ang mag-i-interpret sa kung anong emosyon ang isinagawa sa kanyang braso at sasagot sa pamamagitan ng pagbilog sa salita (emosyon) sa isang listahan. Sa isang testing session magkatapat ang encoder at decoder kung saan mayroong kurtina sa pagitan nila. Sinasabi ng experimenter kung ang mga kalahok ay magkapareha (couple) o hindi magkakilala. Saka isasagawa ang mga emosyon sa mga . Sa kabuuan ay 12 emosyon ang ginamit sa pag-aaral at lahat ito’y isinagawa ng bawat isa sa mga kalahok na encoder. Anim dito ay ang tinatawag na universal emotions: anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness at surprise;   tatlong prosocial emotions: love, gratitude at sympathy; at tatlong self-focused emotions: embarrassment, envy, at pride.

Sa resulta ng pag-aaral, parehong nagawa ng hindi magkakilala at ng magsing-irog (magkapareha) na i-express ang mga universal at prosocial emotions sa pamamagitan lamang ng touch. Subalit tanging mga magsing-irog lamang ang nagawang ma-i-communicate ang self-focused emotions na envy at pride.  Ayon pa kina Thompson & Hampton (2011) ang mga emosyong karaniwang kinalilituhan natin ay iyong mga hindi nagkakalayo (hal., sa valence) at ‘yong magkakatulad ang type of touch na ginagamit at

Bukod dito ay ikinumpara rin sa pag-aaral ang ginamit na paraan ng pag-touch (na nabanggit na sa itaas) kapag ang kapareha at kapag hindi kakilala ang ita-touch. Ito ay isinagawa sa pamamagitan ng pagpapanood ng 10 sa mga video ng naganap na experiment sa “judges”. Ang mga nagsilbing “judges”ay umayon (97% of the time) sa mga coding (type of touch) ng lahat ng “touching-touching” na naganap.  Lumalabas na magkatulad lamang ang uri ng touch na ginagamit ng kapag magkapareha o hindi magkakilala.

Malalaman mo kaya kung ang taong humahawak sa’yo ay kakilala mo o hindi? Oo raw. Oo naman lalo na kung ang iniirog mo ang hahawak sa’yo! Nasa paraan ‘yan ng pag-touching-touching ninyo sa isa eh. Lalo na kung parang may kuryente. 

At dahil diyan, may knock-knock joke ako.

*
Ako: Knock! Knock!
Ikaw: Who’s there?
Ako: Touching-touching.
Ikaw: Touching-touching who?
Ako: It's not about the money, money, money
We don't need your money, money, money
We just wanna make the world dance,
Forget about the price tag
Ain't about the (uh) Touching-touching!
NGEE!






Thompson, E. H., & Hampton, J. A. (2011). The effect of relationship status on communicating emotions through touch. Cognition and emotion , 25 (2), 295-306.
Related Posts Plugin for WordPress, Blogger...